Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorÖhman, Lisa
dc.date.accessioned2013-02-07T06:50:41Z
dc.date.available2013-02-07T06:50:41Z
dc.date.issued2013-02-07
dc.identifier.isbn978-91-628-8623-3
dc.identifier.issn1101-718X
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/2077/32014
dc.description.abstractMany crimes are committed under conditions of darkness, by masked perpetrators or over a phone. In such cases the witnesses’ auditory observations may have a vital role in the investigative phase and in court. Nevertheless, earwitness testimony is a neglected research area. The present thesis investigated earwitnesses’ (i) identification performance for an unfamiliar voice, (ii) memory for the perpetrator’s statement, and (iii) ability to describe the voice. All four studies used the same general setup; exposure to an unfamiliar voice for 40 seconds, and an interview including a seven-voice lineup after a two week delay. High ecological validity was a specific aim across all studies. Study I explored the performance of children aged 7–9 (N = 95), 11–13 (N = 78), and adults (N = 91). Half were exposed to a Target-Present lineup (TP), and half to a Target-Absent lineup (TA). For both types of lineups the participants performed poorly. In the TP condition only the 11–13-year olds (27 % correct) performed above chance level. Furthermore, in the TA condition, all age-groups showed a high willingness to make an identification. Study II investigated the influence of presentation format (direct vs. mobile phone recorded voices) on voice recognition accuracy. The participating adults (N = 165) were assigned randomly to one of the four conditions (Initial exposure: direct vs. mobile phone recorded voice; Lineup presentation: direct vs. mobile phone recorded voices). The overall accuracy for correct identification was 13%, which is expected by chance. Further, the results did not reveal any significant effect of presentation format or lineup format. Study III compared three types of interviews intended to enhance witnesses’ voice memory, as well as content recall. Additionally, an interview protocol developed by the Swedish Security Service, for questioning people that have only heard the perpetrator, was evaluated. After exposure, 11–13-year-olds (N = 119) and adults (N = 93) were interviewed, and returned after two weeks for an additional interview and a lineup. Overall performance for correct identifications was poor (children: 20%, adults: 19%), and an interview shortly after the witnessed event did not seem to help. The Cognitive Interview (vs. the Swedish Security Service protocol) was found to be beneficial for recalling the content of a brief conversation. Study IV investigated the effect of the perpetrator’s tone of voice and time delay on voice recognition accuracy. Further, two types of voice description interviews intended to strengthen the encoding of the voice, were tested. Adults (N = 148) and 11–13-year-olds (N = 160) either heard the perpetrator speak in a normal tone both at encoding and in the lineup, or in an angry tone at encoding and in a normal tone in the lineup. Witnesses were then interviewed about the voice, either with global questions, or by rating voice characteristics. Half of the witnesses were presented with a lineup shortly after the interview and the others after two weeks. Overall, neither age-group performed above chance level (children: 13%, adults: 10%) and only time delay affected accuracy significantly. Children tested immediately performed better (21% correct) compared to those children tested after two weeks (9% correct). Further, voice descriptions were found to be poor. In sum, after testing a total of 949 witnesses under a number of different conditions, the message is clear; voice identification under reasonably realistic conditions is a highly difficult task. Actors in the legal system should therefore treat voice identification evidence with caution. For earwitnesses to be really useful we must find ways of improving their performance for voice identification, content recall, and voice descriptions.sv
dc.language.isoengsv
dc.relation.ispartofseriesDoctoral Dissertationsv
dc.relation.haspartÖhman, L., Eriksson, A., & Granhag, P.A. (2011). Overhearing the planning of a crime: Do adults outperform children as earwitnesses? Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 26, 118 -127. ::doi::10.1007/s11896-010-9076-5sv
dc.relation.haspartÖhman, L., Eriksson, A., & Granhag, P.A. (2010). Mobile phone quality vs. direct quality: How the presentation format affects earwitness identification accuracy. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 2, 161–182.sv
dc.relation.haspartÖhman, L., Eriksson, A., & Granhag, P.A. (Available online: 27 Feb 2012). Enhancing adults’ and children’s earwitness memory: Examining three types of interviews. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law. ::doi::10.1080/13218719.2012.658205sv
dc.relation.haspartÖhman, L., Eriksson, A., & Granhag, P.A. (2013). Angry voices from the past and present: Effects on adults’ and children’s earwitness memory. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 10, 57–70. ::doi::10.1002/jip.1381sv
dc.subjectEarwitnessessv
dc.subjectvoice identificationsv
dc.subjectcontent memorysv
dc.subjectvoice descriptionssv
dc.titleAll Ears: Adults' and Children's Earwitness Testimonysv
dc.typeTexteng
dc.type.svepDoctoral thesiseng
dc.gup.maillisa.ohman@psy.gu.sesv
dc.type.degreeDoctor of Philosophysv
dc.gup.originGöteborgs universitet. Samhällsvetenskapliga fakultetenswe
dc.gup.originUniversity of Gothenburg. Faculty of Social Scienceseng
dc.gup.departmentDepartment of Psychology ; Psykologiska institutionensv
dc.gup.defenceplaceFredagen den 1 mars 2013, kl. 10.00, sal F1, Psykologiska institutionen, Haraldsgatan 1, Göteborgsv
dc.gup.defencedate2013-03-01
dc.gup.dissdb-fakultetSF


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record