dc.contributor.author | Stålhammar, Jacob | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2019-05-16T06:57:05Z | |
dc.date.available | 2019-05-16T06:57:05Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2019-05-16 | |
dc.identifier.isbn | 978-91-7833-420-9 (PRINT) | |
dc.identifier.isbn | 978-91-7833-421-6 (PDF) | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/2077/59540 | |
dc.description.abstract | Cognition comprises all thought processes, from perception to memory.
Neuropsychological tests are the gold standard (= best way) to measure cognition.
However, clinical assessment may at times have to rely on simplified assumptions.
This work addresses potential risks of four such assumptions through
neuropsychological tests and statistical analysis from: a case report (Paper I);
participant data from the Gothenburg Mild Cognitive Impairment study (Papers II,
III); and the Swedish Cardio Pulmonary bioImage Study (SCAPIS Pilot, Paper IV).
Paper I showed transfer effects from memory training may affect memory tests.
Paper II showed that giving free credits for items not administered inflated the scores
of those most impaired in the Boston Naming Test (BNT).
Paper III showed practice effects could not be ruled out in mild cognitive impairment,
and that mean Δ-scores described change better than isolated Δ-scores.
Paper IV showed that administering neuropsychological tests in Swedish to non-native
speakers gave lower results in tests tapping speed and attention, and that vocabulary
testing may enhance precision.
Conclusion: the four assumptions save time at the cost of precision. In the greatest
need for precision, (e.g. for detection of gradual change before manifest loss),
considering the above findings will improve assessments. | sv |
dc.language.iso | eng | sv |
dc.relation.haspart | I. Stålhammar, J., Nordlund, A., Wallin, A. An example of exceptional practice
effects in the verbal domain Neurocase 2015; 21(2):162-8 ::doi::10.1080/13554794.2013.878727 | sv |
dc.relation.haspart | II. Stålhammar, J., Rydén, I., Nordlund, A., Wallin, A.
Boston Naming Test automatic credits inflate scores of nonaphasic mild dementia patients J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2016; 38(4):381-92
::doi::10.1080/13803395.2015.1119254 | sv |
dc.relation.haspart | III. Stålhammar, J., Hellström, P., Joas, E., Göthlin, M., Rolstad, S., Eckerström, C.,
Eckerström, M., Wallin, A. From slow and stable, to abrupt and variable; the
range of mild cognitive impairment-to-dementia neuropsychology change scores
Submitted online 2019-03-03. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult. | sv |
dc.relation.haspart | IV. Stålhammar, J., Hellström, P., Eckerström, C., Wallin, A. Neuropsychological
test performance of middle aged native and non-native Swedish speakers: No
executive advantage. Manuscript. | sv |
dc.subject | Neuropsychology | sv |
dc.subject | practice effects | sv |
dc.subject | mild cognitive impairment | sv |
dc.subject | dementia | sv |
dc.subject | second language effects | sv |
dc.subject | bilingualism | sv |
dc.title | Precision in neuropsychology. Four challenges when using simplified assumptions | sv |
dc.type | text | eng |
dc.type.svep | Doctoral thesis | eng |
dc.gup.mail | jacob.stalhammar@neuro.gu.se | sv |
dc.type.degree | Doctor of Philosophy (Medicine) | sv |
dc.gup.origin | University of Gothenburg. Sahlgrenska Academy | sv |
dc.gup.department | Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology. Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry | sv |
dc.gup.defenceplace | Måndagen den 10 juni 2019, kl. 9.00, Hörsal Arvid Carlsson, Academicum, Medicinaregatan 3, Göteborg | sv |
dc.gup.defencedate | 2019-06-10 | |
dc.gup.dissdb-fakultet | SA | |